Play Video
1
Contemporary Tetrachromist Artist, Concetta Artico talks about "Metamorphosis
Contemporary Tetrachromist Artist, Concetta Artico talks about "Metamorphosis'
::2013/04/24::
Play Video
2
News Coverage of Concetta
News Coverage of Concetta's "Super Vision"
::2014/04/24::
Play Video
3
Concetta Antico - Tetrachromat
Concetta Antico - Tetrachromat
::2013/11/26::
Play Video
4
Color Blindness
Color Blindness
::2013/07/08::
Play Video
5
Impossible Colours
Impossible Colours
::2014/02/19::
Play Video
6
MaЯiUb - Tetrachromacy
MaЯiUb - Tetrachromacy
::2013/03/05::
Play Video
7
sonambient - tetrachromacy
sonambient - tetrachromacy
::2011/11/02::
Play Video
8
Tetrachromacy @ Ravelations Dubai - Aug. 30, 2012
Tetrachromacy @ Ravelations Dubai - Aug. 30, 2012
::2012/08/31::
Play Video
9
How to Pronounce Tetrachromacy
How to Pronounce Tetrachromacy
::2014/08/18::
Play Video
10
tetrachromacy
tetrachromacy
::2013/08/26::
Play Video
11
Tetrachromacy LR:2 Trailer
Tetrachromacy LR:2 Trailer
::2008/12/23::
Play Video
12
Tetrachromacy LR2:U
Tetrachromacy LR2:U
::2008/12/24::
Play Video
13
Concept Insomnia  - "tetrachromacy"
Concept Insomnia - "tetrachromacy"
::2011/04/03::
Play Video
14
How birds and bees can see  UV light and humans can
How birds and bees can see UV light and humans can't
::2011/04/05::
Play Video
15
pasteur "abubygss" official music video
pasteur "abubygss" official music video
::2014/06/18::
Play Video
16
BLKLNTRNS
BLKLNTRNS 'Tetrachromat' Consume Summer Teaser
::2013/04/04::
Play Video
17
Tetrachromat Shimmering
Tetrachromat Shimmering
::2013/12/26::
Play Video
18
Color Theory Intro for Color
Color Theory Intro for Color
::2011/10/20::
Play Video
19
JamesonWinkler2014Aug15
JamesonWinkler2014Aug15
::2014/08/17::
Play Video
20
Concetta Antico featured on San Diego Living
Concetta Antico featured on San Diego Living
::2014/04/22::
Play Video
21
Mark Lyell - Ghost Frequencies
Mark Lyell - Ghost Frequencies
::2013/02/09::
Play Video
22
Mark Lyell - Space In
Mark Lyell - Space In '34 (Pro Mastered Version)
::2013/02/01::
Play Video
23
Mark Lyell - Templeton Woods
Mark Lyell - Templeton Woods
::2013/03/28::
Play Video
24
銀河廃線 - あの夜、僕は汽車から降りた。Digest - Long Ver.
銀河廃線 - あの夜、僕は汽車から降りた。Digest - Long Ver.
::2014/03/19::
Play Video
25
Mark Lyell - Meridian
Mark Lyell - Meridian
::2013/03/28::
Play Video
26
Mark Lyell - Handy Sieves
Mark Lyell - Handy Sieves
::2013/03/28::
Play Video
27
Mark Lyell - East Sussex
Mark Lyell - East Sussex
::2012/10/06::
Play Video
28
Mark Lyell - I Begged Them Not To Destroy It
Mark Lyell - I Begged Them Not To Destroy It
::2013/03/28::
Play Video
29
컬러즈 (Colours) - Chamchi (Black) (Live)
컬러즈 (Colours) - Chamchi (Black) (Live)
::2014/03/25::
Play Video
30
Mark Lyell - Truly Obfuscate The Contended Disciples (Pro Mastered Version)
Mark Lyell - Truly Obfuscate The Contended Disciples (Pro Mastered Version)
::2013/02/01::
Play Video
31
Mark Lyell - The Day The Stranger Died
Mark Lyell - The Day The Stranger Died
::2013/03/28::
Play Video
32
Mark Lyell - Hallavell
Mark Lyell - Hallavell
::2013/03/28::
Play Video
33
Mark Lyell - Poltergeist
Mark Lyell - Poltergeist
::2013/02/05::
Play Video
34
Mark Lyell - Space In
Mark Lyell - Space In '34
::2012/05/28::
Play Video
35
Mark Lyell - Mind Aneurism
Mark Lyell - Mind Aneurism
::2013/03/28::
Play Video
36
Mark Lyell - The Bus To Glenelg
Mark Lyell - The Bus To Glenelg
::2013/03/28::
Play Video
37
Mark Lyell - y.gods
Mark Lyell - y.gods'l.con.u
::2013/02/05::
Play Video
38
Mark Lyell - Truly Obfuscate The Contented Disciples
Mark Lyell - Truly Obfuscate The Contented Disciples
::2012/07/20::
Play Video
39
Mark Lyell - King Arthur [Rough Mix]
Mark Lyell - King Arthur [Rough Mix]
::2012/06/05::
Play Video
40
Mark Lyell - Jupidelphia
Mark Lyell - Jupidelphia
::2013/03/28::
Play Video
41
5 Interesting Science Facts #1   HIV, Chernobyl and Margaret Thatcher
5 Interesting Science Facts #1 HIV, Chernobyl and Margaret Thatcher
::2014/02/13::
Play Video
42
Mark Lyell - Valentich
Mark Lyell - Valentich
::2013/03/28::
Play Video
43
Mark Lyell - The Somerton Man
Mark Lyell - The Somerton Man
::2013/03/28::
Play Video
44
Mark Lyell - Some Tapping
Mark Lyell - Some Tapping
::2012/05/31::
Play Video
45
Mark Lyell - AJ10
Mark Lyell - AJ10
::2013/02/09::
Play Video
46
Mark Lyell - Some Maiden Riffs
Mark Lyell - Some Maiden Riffs
::2012/05/31::
Play Video
47
Mark Lyell - Jamaican Thrash
Mark Lyell - Jamaican Thrash
::2012/07/25::
Play Video
48
Mark Lyell - Randeity
Mark Lyell - Randeity
::2013/02/18::
Play Video
49
Mark Lyell - Evidence (Part 1)
Mark Lyell - Evidence (Part 1)
::2012/07/25::
Play Video
50
MENTAL METAL v/s MADD NADD - Ravelations Dubai - 5th April, 2013
MENTAL METAL v/s MADD NADD - Ravelations Dubai - 5th April, 2013
::2013/04/06::
NEXT >>
RESULTS [51 .. 101]
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Jump to: navigation, search
The four pigments in a bird's cones (in this example, estrildid finches) extend the range of color vision into the ultraviolet.[1]

Tetrachromacy is the condition of possessing four independent channels for conveying color information, or possessing four different types of cone cells in the eye. Organisms with tetrachromacy are called tetrachromats.

In tetrachromatic organisms, the sensory color space is four-dimensional, meaning that to match the sensory effect of arbitrarily chosen spectra of light within their visible spectrum requires mixtures of at least four different primary colors.

Tetrachromacy is demonstrated among several species of birds, fish, amphibians, reptiles and insects.[2][3] It was also the normal condition of most mammals in the past; a genetic change made the majority of species of this class eventually lose two of their four cones.[4][5]

Physiology[edit]

The normal explanation of tetrachromacy is that the organism's retina contains four types of higher-intensity light receptors (called cone cells in vertebrates as opposed to rod cells which are lower intensity light receptors) with different absorption spectra. This means the animal may see wavelengths beyond those of a typical human being's eyesight, and may be able to distinguish colors that to a human appear to be identical. Species with tetrachromatic color vision have a small physiological advantage over rival species.[6]

Examples[edit]

Fish[edit]

The goldfish (Carassius auratus auratus)[7] and zebrafish (Danio rerio)[8] are examples of tetrachromats, containing cone cells sensitive for red, green, blue and ultraviolet light.

Birds[edit]

Some species of birds such as the Zebra Finch and the Columbidae use the ultraviolet wavelength (300–400 nm) specific to tetrachromatic color vision as a tool during mate selection and foraging.[9] When selecting for mates, ultraviolet plumage and skin coloration show a high level of selection.[10] A typical bird eye will respond to wavelengths from about 300 to 700 nm. In terms of frequency, this corresponds to a band in the vicinity of 430–1000 THz.

Insects[edit]

Foraging insects can see wavelengths that flowers reflect (ranging from 300 nm to 700 nm[11][12]). Pollination being a mutualistic relationship, foraging insects and plants have coevolved, both increasing wavelength range: in perception (pollinators), in reflection and variation (flower colors).[6] Directional selection has led plants to display increasingly diverse amounts of color variations extending into the ultraviolet color scale, thus attracting higher levels of pollinators.[6] Some pollinators may use tetrachromatic color vision to increase and maintain a higher foraging success rate over their trichromatic competitors.[citation needed]

Possibility of human tetrachromats[edit]

Humans and closely related primates normally have three types of cone cells and are therefore trichromats (animals with three different cones). However, at low light intensities the rod cells may contribute to color vision, giving a small region of tetrachromacy in the color space.[13]

In humans, two cone cell pigment genes are located on the sex X chromosome, the classical type 2 opsin genes OPN1MW and OPN1MW2. It has been suggested that as women have two different X chromosomes in their cells, some of them could be carrying some variant cone cell pigments, thereby possibly being born as full tetrachromats and having four different simultaneously functioning kinds of cone cells, each type with a specific pattern of responsiveness to different wavelengths of light in the range of the visible spectrum.[14] One study suggested that 2–3% of the world's women might have the kind of fourth cone that lies between the standard red and green cones, giving, theoretically, a significant increase in color differentiation.[15] Another study suggests that as many as 50% of women and 8% of men may have four photopigments and corresponding increased chromatic discrimination in comparison to trichromats.[14] In June 2012, after 20 years of study of women with four cones (non-functional tetrachromats), neuroscientist Dr. Gabriele Jordan identified a woman (subject cDa29) who was able to detect a greater variety of colors than trichromatic ones, corresponding with a functional tetrachromat (or true tetrachromat).[16][17]

Variation in cone pigment genes is widespread in most human populations, but the most prevalent and pronounced tetrachromacy would derive from female carriers of major red/green pigment anomalies, usually classed as forms of "color blindness" (protanomaly or deuteranomaly). The biological basis for this phenomenon is X-inactivation of heterozygotic alleles for retinal pigment genes, which is the same mechanism that gives the majority of female new-world monkeys trichromatic vision.

In humans, preliminary visual processing occurs within the neurons of the retina. It is not known how these nerves would respond to a new color channel, that is, whether they could handle it separately or just combine it in with an existing channel. Visual information leaves the eye by way of the optic nerve; it is not known whether the optic nerve has the spare capacity to handle a new color channel. A variety of final image processing takes place in the brain; it is not known how the various areas of the brain would respond if presented with a new color channel.

Mice, which normally have only two cone pigments, can be engineered to express a third cone pigment, and appear to demonstrate increased chromatic discrimination,[18] arguing against some of these obstacles; however, the original publication's claims about plasticity in the optic nerve have also been disputed.[19]

Humans cannot perceive UV light directly because the lens of the eye blocks most light in the wavelength range of 300–400 nm; shorter wavelengths are blocked by the cornea.[20] Nevertheless, the photoreceptors of the retina are sensitive to near UV light and people lacking a lens (a condition known as aphakia) perceive near UV light as whitish blue or whitish-violet, probably because all three types of cones are roughly equally sensitive to UV light, but blue cones a bit more.[21]

See also[edit]

References[edit]

  1. ^ Figure data, uncorrected absorbance curve fits, from Hart NS, Partridge JC, Bennett ATD and Cuthill IC (2000) Visual pigments, cone oil droplets and ocular media in four species of estrildid finch. Journal of Comparative Physiology A186 (7-8): 681-694.
  2. ^ Goldsmith, Timothy H. (2006). "What Birds See". Scientific American (July 2006): 69–75. 
  3. ^ Wilkie, Susan E.; Vissers, Peter M. A. M.; Das, Debipriya; Degrip, Willem J.; Bowmaker, James K.; Hunt, David M. (1998). "The molecular basis for UV vision in birds: spectral characteristics, cDNA sequence and retinal localization of the UV-sensitive visual pigment of the budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus)". Biochemical Journal 330 (Pt 1): 541–47. PMC 1219171. PMID 9461554. 
  4. ^ Jacobs, G. H. (2009). "Evolution of colour vision in mammals". Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 364 (1531): 2957–2967. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0039. PMC 2781854. PMID 19720656. 
  5. ^ Arrese, C. A. et al. (2005). "Cone topography and spectral sensitivity in two potentially trichromatic marsupials, the quokka (Setonix brachyurus) and quenda (Isoodon obesulus)". Proc. Biol. Sci. 272 (1565): 791–796. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.3009. PMC 1599861. PMID 15888411. 
  6. ^ a b c Backhaus, W., Kliegl, R., Werner, J.S. (1998). Color vision: perspective from different disciplines. pp. 163–182. 
  7. ^ Neumeyer, Christa (1988). Das Farbensehen des Goldfisches: Eine verhaltensphysiologische Analyse. G. Thieme. ISBN 313718701X. 
  8. ^ Robinson, J.; Schmitt, E.A.; Harosi, F.I.; Reece, R.J.; Dowling, J.E. (1993). "Zebrafish ultraviolet visual pigment: absorption spectrum, sequence, and localization". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 90 (13): 6009–6012. doi:10.1073/pnas.90.13.6009. PMC 46856. PMID 8327475. 
  9. ^ Bennett, Andrew T. D.; Cuthill, Innes C.; Partridge, Julian C.; Maier, Erhard J. (1996). "Ultraviolet vision and mate choice in zebra finches". Nature 380 (6573): 433–435. doi:10.1038/380433a0. 
  10. ^ Bennett, Andrew T. D.; Théry, Marc (2007). "Avian Color Vision and Coloration: Multidisciplinary Evolutionary Biology". The American Naturalist 169 (S1): S1–S6. doi:10.1086/510163. ISSN 0003-0147. 
  11. ^ Markha, K. R.; Bloor, S. J.; Nicholson, R.; Rivera, R.; Shemluck, M.; Kevan, P. G.; Michener, C. (2004). "Black flower coloration in wild lisianthius nigrescens". Z Naturforsch C 59c (9-10): 625–630. PMID 15540592. 
  12. ^ Backhaus, W.; Kliegl, R.; Werner, J. S., eds. (1998). Colour Vision: Perspectives from Different Disciplines. pp. 45–78. 
  13. ^ Hansjochem Autrum and Richard Jung (1973). Integrative Functions and Comparative Data. 7 (3). Springer-Verlag. p. 226. ISBN 978-0-387-05769-9. 
  14. ^ a b Jameson, K. A., Highnote, S. M., & Wasserman, L. M. (2001). "Richer color experience in observers with multiple photopigment opsin genes" (PDF). Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 8 (2): 244–261. doi:10.3758/BF03196159. PMID 11495112. 
  15. ^ Roth, Mark (13 September 2006). "Some women may see 100,000,000 colors, thanks to their genes". Pittsburgh Post-Gazette. 
  16. ^ Didymus, JohnThomas (Jun 19, 2012), "Scientists find woman who sees 99 million more colors than others", Digital Journal 
  17. ^ Jordan, G.; Mollon, J. D. (1993). "A study of women heterozygous for colour deficiencies". Vision research 33 (11): 1495–1508. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(93)90143-k. PMID 8351822.  edit
  18. ^ Jacobs et al.; Williams, GA; Cahill, H; Nathans, J (23 March 2007). "Emergence of Novel Color Vision in Mice Engineered to Express a Human Cone Photopigment". Science 315 (5819): 1723–1725. doi:10.1126/science.1138838. PMID 17379811. 
  19. ^ Makous, W. (12 October 2007). "Comment on "Emergence of Novel Color Vision in Mice Engineered to Express a Human Cone Photopigment"". Science 318 (5848): 196. doi:10.1126/science.1146084. PMID 17932271. 
  20. ^ M A Mainster (2006). "Violet and blue light blocking intraocular lenses: photoprotection versus photoreception". British Journal of Ophthalmology 90 (6): 784–792. doi:10.1136/bjo.2005.086553. PMC 1860240. 
  21. ^ Hambling, David (29 May 2002). "Let the light shine in". The Guardian. 

External links[edit]

Ágnes Holba & B. Lukács

"Human tetrachromacy is extensively discussed in the literature recently, and it seems that we already understand the genetic background of at least some kinds of it. However its mathematics has not yet been analysed in details. Here we are going to discuss the dimensionality and topology of the colour space."

Wikipedia content is licensed under the GFDL License
Powered by YouTube
LEGAL
  • Mashpedia © 2014